基于多客体追踪的视觉空间注意研究
摘要
在一定时间内排除干扰,持续追踪相关对象是一项具有挑战性的认知任务。本文在多客体追踪任务的内涵及研究范
式的基础上,基于视觉空间注意的视角探讨多目标追踪过程中个体的核心认知能力,着重分析动态场景中视觉空间
注意的心理机制,并从视觉空间注意的局限性探讨多客体追踪的影响因素,最后,基于多客体追踪任务学习的必要
性,提出改善个体的多客体追踪任务绩效的策略,包括训练策略、感觉整合策略和分组策略。
关键词
全文:
PDF参考
[1]Pichlmeier Sebastian & Pfeiffer Till.(2021). Attentional
capture in multiple object tracking. Journal of vision, (8):16, 1-20.
[2]Luo, W., Xing, J., Milan, A., Zhang, X., Liu, W.,
& Kim, T.-K. (2021). Multiple object tracking: A literature
review. Artificial Intelligence, 293, 103448.
[3]Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking
multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking
mechanism. Spat Vis, 3(3), 179-197.
[4]Scholl, B. J.. (2009). What have we learned
about attention from multiple-object tracking (and vice
versa)?. Computation.
[5]Wahn, B., & König, P. (2015a). Audition and vision
share spatial attentional resources, yet attentional load does
not disrupt audiovisual integration. Frontiers in Psychology,
6(1084), 1084.
[6]Wahn, B., & König, P. (2015b). Vision and haptics
share spatial attentional resources and visuotactile integration is
not affected by high attentional load. Multisensory Research,
28(3-4), 371-392.
[7]Doran, M. M., & Hoffman, J. E. (2010). The role
of visual attention in multiple object tracking: evidence from
erps. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72(1), 33-52.
[8]Wahn, B., & König, P. (2016). Attentional resource
allocation in visuotactile processing depends on the task, but
optimal visuotactile integration does not depend on attentional
resources. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 10(727).
[9]Jahn, G., Wendt, J., Lotze, M., Papenmeier, F., &
Huff, M.. (2012). Brain activation during spatial updating
and attentive tracking of moving targets. Brain & Cognition,
78(2), 0-113.
[10]Ungerleider, L., & Pessoa, L. (2008). What and
where pathways. Scholarpedia, 3(11), 5342.
[11]Drew, T., Horowitz, T. S., & Vogel, E. K.. (2013).
Swapping or dropping? electrophysiological measures of
difficulty during multiple object tracking. Cognition, 126(2).
[12]Culham, J. C.. (2001). Attention response functions:
characterizing brain areas using fmri activation during
parametric variations of attentional load. Neuron, 32.
[13]Howe, P. D., Horowitz, T. S., Akos Morocz,
I., Wolfe, J., & Livingstone, M. S.. (2009). Using fmri to
distinguish components of the multiple object tracking task.
Journal of Vision, 9(4), 10-10.
[14]Jovicich, J., Peters, R. J., Koch, C., Braun, J.,
Chang, L., & Ernst, T.. (2001). Brain areas specific for
attentional load in a motion-tracking task. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(8), 1048-1058.
[15]Datta, R., & Deyoe, E. A.. (2009). I know where
you are secretly attending! the topography of human visual
attention revealed with fMRI. Vision Research, 49(10),
1037-1044.
[16]Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Datta, R., Lewis, J. W., &
Deyoe, E. A.. (2009). The topography of visuospatial attention as
revealed by a novel visual field mapping technique. CognitiveNeuroscience Journal of, 21(7), 1447-1460.
[17]Sanefuji, M., Yamashita, H., Torio, M., Katsuki,
D., Akamine, S., & Ishizaki, Y., et al. (2018). A rightward
saccade to an unexpected stimulus as a marker for lateralised
visuospatial attention. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 7562.
[18]Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L.. (2002). Control
of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.
Nature reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 215-229.
[19]Newman, D. P., O’Connell, Redmond G., &
Bellgrove, M. A.. (2013). Linking time-on-task, spatial bias and
hemispheric activation asymmetry: a neural correlate of rightward
attention drift. Neuropsychologia, 51(7), 1215-1223.
[20]Spencer, J. P., Barich, K., Goldberg, J., & Perone,
S. (2012). Behavioral dynamics and neural grounding of a
dynamic field theory of multi-object tracking. Journal of
Integrative Neuroscience, 11(3), 339-362.
[21]Botterill, K., Allen, R., & Mcgeorge, P. (2011).
Multiple-object tracking: the binding of spatial location and
featural identity. Experimental Psychology, 58(3), 196.
[22]Fencsik, D. E., Klieger, S. B., & Horowitz, T. S.
(2007). The role of location and motion information in the
tracking and recovery of moving objects. Percept Psychophys,
69(4), 567-577.
[23]余湘娥.(2013).背景和客体的相似性对多客体
追踪的影响.(Doctoral dissertation,浙江大学).
[24]Liu, Y., J. Bengson, H. Huang, et al. 2016. Topdown mod-ulation of neural activity in anticipatory visual
attention: control mechanisms revealed by simultaneous
EEG–fMRI.Cereb. Cortex 26: 517-529.
[25]Dux, P. E., Ivanoff, J., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R..
(2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing
with time-resolved fmri. Neuron, 52(6), 1109-1120.
[26]Molloy, K., Griffiths, T. D., Chait, M., & Lavie,
N.. (2015). Inattentional deafness: visual load leads to timespecific suppression of auditory evoked responses. Journal of
Neuroscience, 35(49), 16046-16054.
[27]Raveh, D., & Lavie, N.. (2015). Load-induced
inattentional deafness. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,
77(2), 483-492.
[28]Matusz, P. J., Broadbent, H., Ferrari, J., Forrest, B.,
Merkley, R., & Scerif, G.. (2015). Multi-modal distraction:
insights from children’s limited attention. Cognition,
136(136), 156-165.
[29]Skewes, J. C., Skewes, L., Michael, J., & Konvalinka,
I. (2015). Synchronised and complementary coordination
mechanisms in an asymmetric joint aiming task. Experimental
Brain Research, 233(2), 551-565.
[30]Rigoli, L., & Spivey, M. J. (2015). Real-Time
Language Processing as Embodied and Embedded in Joint
Action. Attention and Vision in Language Processing.
[31]Wahn, B., & König, P. (2017). Can limitations of
visuospatial attention be circumvented? a review. Frontiers in
Psychology, 8, 1896.
[32]Blankenship Tashauna L,Strong Roger W & Kibbe
Melissa M.(2020).Development of multiple object tracking via
multifocal attention. Developmental psychology, 56(9).
[33]Giraudet, L., Berenger, M., Imbert, J.-P.,
Tremblay, S., and Causse, M. (2014).“Inattentional deafness
in simulated air traffic control tasks: a behavioral and p300
analysis,”in 5th International Conference on Applied Human
Factors and Ergonomics (Kraków).
[34]König, S. U., Schumann, F., Keyser, J., Goeke,
C., Krause, C., Wache, S., et al. (2016). Learning new
sensorimotor contingencies: effects of long-term use of
sensory augmentation on the brain and conscious perception.
PLoS ONE 11:e0166647.
[35]Maidenbaum, S., Abboud, S., & Amedi, A.. (2014).
Sensory substitution: closing the gap between basic research
and widespread practical visual rehabilitation. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 41(Sp. Iss. SI), 3-15.
[36]Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006). Enumeration
versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game
players. Cognition, 101(1), 217-245.
[37]Makovski, T., Vázquez, G. A., & Jiang, Y. V.
(2008). Visual learning in multiple-object tracking. PLoS
ONE, 3(5), e2228-.
[38]Tal, M., Vázquez Gustavo A., Jiang, Y. V., &
Herzog, M. H.. (2008). Visual learning in multiple-object
tracking. PLoS ONE, 3(5), e2228-.
[39]Helbig, H. B., & Ernst, M. O.. (2008). Visualhaptic cue weighting is independent of modality-specific
attention. Journal of Vision, 8(1), 21.1-16.[40]Gibney, K. D., Enimielen, A., Eggleston, B. A.,
Nunes, S. R., Kerkhoff, W. G., & Dean, C. L., et al. (2017).
Visual distractors disrupt audiovisual integration regardless of
stimulus complexity. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 11.
[41]Ten Oever, S., Romei, V., Van Atteveldt, N.,
Soto-Faraco, S., Murray, M. M., & Matusz, P. J.. (2016). The
cogs (context, object, and goals) in multisensory processing.
Experimental Brain Research, 234(5), 1307-1323.
[42]Macaluso, E., Noppeney, U., Talsma, D., Vercillo,
T., Hartcher-O’Brien, J., and Adam, R. (2016). The curious
incident of attention in multisensory integration: bottom-up vs.
top-down. Multisensory Research. 29, 557-583.
[43]Tang, X., Wu, J., & Shen, Y.. (2015). The
interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and
exogenous attention. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,
S0149763415002730.
[44]Yantis Steven. (1992). Multielement visual tracking:
Attention and perceptual organization. Cognitive Psychology,
24(3), 295-230.
[45]Yin, J., Ding, X., Zhou, J., Shui, R., Li, X., & Shen,
M. (2013). Social grouping: Perceptual grouping of objects
by cooperative but not competitive relationships in dynamic
chase. Cognition, 129(1), 194-204.
[46]黄媛.(2016).多客体追踪中基于社会关系信息
的分组效应:来自对侧延迟活动的证据(硕士学位论文,
浙江大学).
Refbacks
- 当前没有refback。